
was a large lean man, with that Henry-Ford-Wilbur-Wright kind of face that usually goes with the
slow, taciturn speech of the sun-tanned pioneer. It didn't in Professor Hinckelberg's case. He
could talk like an L.P. record on a 78 turntable. In about ten seconds we'd discovered that he was
a zoologist on leave of absence from a North Virginia college, that he was attached to the Office
of Naval Research on some project to do with plankton, that he was tickled pink with London and
even liked English beer, that he'd heard about us through a letter in Science but couldn't believe
we were true, that Stevenson was O.K. but if the Democrats wanted to get back they'd better
import Winston, that he'd like to know what the heck was wrong with all our telephone call boxes
and could he retrieve the small fortune in coppers of which they had mulcted him, that there
seemed to be a lot of empty glasses around and how about filling them up, boys?
On the whole the Professor's shock-tactics were well received, but when he made a momentary
pause for breath I thought to myself "Harry'd better look out. This guy can talk rings round him."
I glanced at Purvis, who was only a few feet away from me, and saw that his lips were pursed into
a slight frown. I sat back luxuriously and awaited results.
As it was a fairly busy evening, it was quite sometime before Professor Hinckelberg had been
introduced to everybody. Harry, usually so forward at meeting celebrities, seemed to be keeping
out of the way. But eventually he was cornered by Arthur Vincent, who acts as informal club
secretary and makes sure that everyone signs the visitors' book.
"I'm sure you and Harry will have a lot to talk about" said Arthur, in a burst of innocent
enthusiasm. "You're both scientists, aren't you? And Harry's had some most extraordinary things
happen to him. Tell the Professor about the time you found that U 235 in your letterbox. . . ."
"I don't think," said Harry, a trifle too hastily, "that Professor—ah—Hinckelberg wants to listen
to my little adventure. I'm sure he must have a lot to tell us."
I've puzzled my head about that reply a good deal since then. It wasn't in character. Usually, with
an opening like this, Purvis was up and away. Perhaps he was seizing up the enemy, waiting for
the Professor to make the first mistake, and then swooping in to the kill. If that was the
explanation, he'd misjudged his man. He never had a chance, for Professor Hinckelberg made a
jet-assisted take-off and was immediately in full flight.
"Odd you should mention that," he said. "I've just been dealing with a most remarkable case. It's
one of these things that can't be written up as a proper scientific paper, and this seems a good tune
to get it off my chest. I can't often do that, because of this darned security—but so far no-one's
gotten round to classifying Dr. Grinnell's experiments, so I'll talk about them while I can."
Grinnell, it seemed was one of the many scientists trying to interpret the behaviour of the nervous
system in terms of electrical circuits. He had started, as Grey Walter, Shannon and others had
done, by making models that could reproduce the simpler actions of living creatures. His greatest
success in this direction had been a mechanical cat that could chase mice and could land on its
feet when dropped from a height. Very quickly, however, he had branched off in another
direction owing to his discovery of what he called "neural induction". This was, to simplify it
greatly, nothing less than a method of actually controlling the behaviour of animals.
It had been known for many years that all the processes that take place in the mind are
accompanied by the production of minute electric currents, and for a long time it has been
possible to record these complex fluctuations —though their exact interpretation is still unknown.
Grinnell had not attempted the intricate task of analysis; what he had done was a good deal
simpler, though its achievement was still complicated enough. He had attached his recording
device to various animals, and thus been able to build up a small library, if one could call it that,
of electrical impulses associated with their behaviour. One pattern of voltage might correspond to
a movement to the right, another with traveling in a circle, another with complete stillness, and so
on. That was an interesting enough achievement, but Grinnell had not stopped there. By "playing
back" the impulses he had recorded, he could compel his subjects to repeat their previous
actions— whether they wanted to or not. That such a thing might be possible in theory almost any
neurologist would admit, but few would have believed that it could be done in practice owing to the