A spectrum of complexity uncovers Dunbars number and other leaps in social structure Mart n Saavedra1 2Jorge Mira3Alberto P Mu nuzuri1 2and Lu s F Seoane4 5

2025-04-30 1 0 1.31MB 13 页 10玖币
侵权投诉
A spectrum of complexity uncovers Dunbar’s number and other leaps in
social structure
Mart´ın Saavedra,1, 2 Jorge Mira,3Alberto P Mu˜nuzuri,1, 2 and Lu´ıs F Seoane4, 5
1Group of Nonlinear Physics. Universidade de Santiago de Compostela, 15782, Santiago de Compostela, Spain.
2Galician Center for Mathematical Research and Technology (CITMAga), 15782 Santiago de Compostela, Spain
3Departamento de F´ısica Aplicada and iMATUS, Universidade de Santiago de Compostela, 15782, Santiago de Compostela,
Spain.
4Systems Biology Department, Spanish National Center for Biotechnology (CSIC), C/ Darwin 3, 28049 Madrid, Spain.
5Grupo Interdisciplinar de Sistemas Complejos (GISC), Madrid, Spain.
Social dynamics are shaped by each person’s actions, as well as by collective trends that emerge
when individuals are brought together. These latter kind of influences escape anyone’s control.
They are, instead, dominated by aggregate societal properties such as size, polarization, cohesion,
or hierarchy. Such features add nuance and complexity to social structure, and might be present,
or not, for societies of different sizes. How do societies become more complex? Are there specific
scales at which they are reorganized into emergent entities? In this paper we introduce the
social complexity spectrum, a methodological tool, inspired by theoretical considerations about
dynamics on complex networks, that addresses these questions empirically. We use as a probe
a sociolinguistic process that has unfolded over decades within the north-western Spanish region
of Galicia, across populations of varied sizes. We estimate how societal complexity increases
monotonously with population size; and how specific scales stand out, at which complexity would
build up faster. These scales are noted as dips in our spectra, similarly to missing wavelengths in
light spectroscopy. Also, ‘red-’ and ‘blue-shifts’ take place as the general population shifted from
more rural to more urban settings. These shifts help us sharpen our observations. Besides specific
results around social complexity build-up, our work introduces a powerful tool to be applied in
further study cases.
Keywords: social complexity, Dunbar number, social networks, social dynamics, linguistics
I. INTRODUCTION
If we put together grains of sand, one by one, at some
point they become a mountain. When does that happen?
Daniel Dennett reminds us that such questions might not
have a clear-cut answer, and teaches us how to live with
the ensuing scale of greys [1]. Yet some complex systems
often change radically as their size crosses certain thresh-
olds. At those points, emerging behaviors start shaping
the fate of the whole, overriding the more straightfor-
ward dynamics of the parts. Then, as put beautifully by
William P. Anderson, “More is Different” [2].
As humans, we contemplate the duality of our auton-
omy and of belonging to an eusocial species. Tensions
between both levels of Darwinian selection (the organ-
ismal and the societal) largely underlie our conflicting
nature [3]. Individually, every modern human has likely
experienced ‘being dragged by the masses’. But, when
does a gathering of people become an emergent entity?
How many such transitions might take place as societies
grow? Are these processes gradual, as with mountains
and grains of sand? Or can we spot relevant scales at
which the dynamics of human behavior is altered by
emerging levels of complexity?
We tackle these questions empirically through a pro-
cess of opinion dynamics that has played out over social
groups of different complexity. Specifically, we tend to
socioliguistic processes of language shift that have oc-
curred in the Spanish Autonomous region of Galicia over
the last 100 years [4]. In this process, the vernacular,
Galician, trended to be substituted by Castilian Span-
ish. Both are romance languages with high mutual intel-
ligibility, which might enable long-term bilingualism and
coexistence of both tongues [5]. These dynamics have un-
folded simultaneously, at different speeds, in a range of
population centers, from very rural ones to larger cities.
Galicia presents quite unusual demographics: It covers
about 6% of the territory of Spain and houses a similar
percentage of the country’s population. Notwithstand-
ing, Galicia contains roughly a half of all 60 000 Spanish
Singular Population Entities (SPEs, defined as any unit
such as villages, cities, etc.). Among these, as of 2016,
27 000 Galician SPEs had less than 100 inhabitants [6].
This gives us a great sampling of social processes hap-
pening on communities with different structures. If we
assume that SPEs of different sizes constitute underlying
social groups of varying complexity, we are provided with
a unique opportunity to study at what emerging social
scales human behavior is altered in a noticeable way.
Social exchanges happen within a social substrate, of-
ten modeled as a graph or network. In them, people
are nodes, and edges represent existing interactions—
whether virtual or of physical contact. Networks can
have different overall structures—e.g., small world [7],
hierarchical or more horizontal [8], etc. Distinct network
structures enable or hinder the unfolding of different phe-
arXiv:2210.15322v1 [physics.soc-ph] 27 Oct 2022
2
FIG. 1 A complex network structure might slow down social dynamics. In [11] it is observed that certain opinion
dynamics can take longer to reach their absorbing steady state due to a complex structure of the underlying social network.
This figure illustrates a mechanism behind this phenomenon. In a clique (top), which is a simple kind of graph, all network
individuals interact with each-other. This enables that the average opinion observed by the i-th individual equals the actual
average opinion across the whole group, hxii=hxi. Conflicting views are resolved as quickly as possible (a red dashed vertical
line marks when the steady state is reached in each case). In random, yet unstructured networks (middle), which are fairly
simple as well, not everybody is connected with everybody else. However, a node’s neighbors are relatively well distributed
across the network, thus its perceived average opinion comes from a good-enough sampling of the group’s state, hxii' hxi.
Dynamics are resolved only slightly more slowly. In complex, structured networks, individuals cluster locally with few nodes
acting as gate-keepers of larger groups (bottom). Individuals inside each group perceive averaged opinions that might depart
from the network’s consensus, hxii6=hxi. Hence, opinion dynamics are slowed-down as bottlenecks prevent the immediate
invasion of each cluster.
nomena, such as opinion dynamics, epidemic spread, co-
ordination towards a goal, etc. For example a sufficiently
sparse network of physical contacts can halt the spread
of a virus—as confinement measures during the recent
pandemic have illustrated. Less trivially, Darwinian evo-
lution can be accelerated if it happens over networks with
specific shapes [9, 10].
A similar interplay between network structure and dy-
namics can apply to other social processes—specifically,
to opinion dynamics such as the decision to keep or
change your tongue. In a computational study, Toivonen
et al. [11] showed how certain classes of opinion dynamics
have longer relaxation times in more complex networks,
while the same processes converge faster to their steady
states in simpler graphs. In more trivial networks (Fig. 1,
top and middle), each individual samples accurately the
social group’s average opinion, and dynamics can be re-
solved quickly. In more complex graphs (Fig. 1, bottom)
nodes of similar opinion can form clusters guarded by
gatekeepers that hardly flip their opinions. These clus-
ters become difficult to penetrate. Hence, some individ-
uals are cut out of the emerging consensus, and conver-
gence to a homogeneous opinion is hindered. Inspired by
this, it was found empirically that sociolinguistic dynam-
ics in Galicia have unfolded faster in more rural areas,
and slower in more urban ones [4].
This last study assumed that urban communities had
a more complex social structure, and it used the thresh-
old of 5000 inhabitants to consider a SPE as urban. This
choice was based on Spanish legislation [12] that demands
that counties (which usually include several SPEs) over
that size present a series of structures and services (a pub-
lic park and library, a market place, and a waste manage-
ment system), potentially marking a jump in social net-
work complexity. Further demands of urban equipment
do not happen below 5 000 inhabitants, or above until
20 000 and 50 000 inhabitants. However, can we relax
this definition of urban and come up with a more organic
way to find salient leaps in social complexity? Perhaps,
if such changes in complexity affect ongoing opinion dy-
namics (as those simulated by Toivonen et al. [11]), we
can look at empirical data of language shift to spot the
relevant scales at which complexity builds up.
In this paper we study this possibility. We try a
range of scales (based on population sizes), and check
whether each scale is a good separation between simple
versus complex social networks. Following [11], our test is
whether sociolinguistic dynamics tend to play out faster
in ‘simpler’ networks. We measure this through correla-
tions between a region’s purported complexity and the
rate at which sociolinguistic dynamics have unfolded in
that area. This renders a kind of ‘spectrogram’ in which
dips are visible at population scales with non-trivial cor-
relations (much like a spectral line is missing in optical
spectroscopy when light traverses specific chemical com-
pounds that absorb a specific wavelength). We find two
salient scales that, according to our criterion, would sepa-
rate simpler from more complex social networks. Our re-
sults suggest that at those scales some emergent compo-
nent takes over and alters the pace at which sociolinguis-
tic dynamics play out. One of these scales corresponds to
the threshold used in [4], which was based on urban plan-
3
ning. The other one, more prominent, happens at much
smaller community sizes (200 people). It does not cor-
relate with any feature marked by Spanish law. Rather,
its proximity to Dunbar’s number (an empirical—yet de-
bated [13–15]—cognitive limit to the number of relation-
ships that animals can have [16–23]) suggests an organic
emergence of social complexity.
In Sec. II.A we describe the sociolinguistic dynamics
of interest to us and the empirical data available. In Sec.
II.B we introduce the mathematical model that we fit to
the data. The fitting procedure is explained in Sec. II.C.
We use the resulting model parameters to estimate the
rate at which the dynamics unfold in each geographical
region. In Sec. III.A we introduce our social complex-
ity spectrum. We explain how we define separations be-
tween potentially simple and potentially complex social
networks, and how we evaluate the goodness of these sep-
arations. Secs. III.A and III.B contain our main results.
Namely, that we identify two outstanding scales at which
leaps, or buildups of social complexity would happen ac-
cording to our criterion. Sec. III.C further explores the
spectrum of social complexity as a methodological tool.
Similarly to physics and optical spectroscopy, we observe
‘red-’ and ‘blue-shifts’ as the overall demographics has
changed over two decades. We illustrate how this can
help us refine the separation of simple and complex so-
cial networks. We wrap up the paper with a discussion of
our findings in Sec. IV. We further argue that the “com-
plexity spectrum” might be a powerful tool to uncover
scales of social relevance when applied to similar dynam-
ics.
II. METHODS
A. Sociolinguistic dynamics and data
We investigate the emergence of singular scales of so-
cial complexity by looking at how certain sociolinguis-
tic dynamics have unfolded at different speeds in regions
that, potentially, contain distinctly complex social net-
works. The dynamics that we use as a proxy is the co-
existence of Castillian Spanish and Galician. Both these
tongues are romance languages that coexist in the Au-
tonomous Community of Galicia, in north-western Spain.
Mutual intelligibility between them is large, allowing
broad bilingual communities and, potentially, a sustained
coexistence between the two of them. While Galician is
the vernacular, a shift towards Castillian Spanish has
been underway for centuries, and has been especially ac-
centuated during the 20th century.
The Galician Statistical Office (Instituto Galego de Es-
tat´ıstica, IGE) has tracked language use in different Gali-
cian regions and across demographic groups. In peri-
odic polls, informants would self-assess their language use
as ‘only Galician’, ‘mostly Galician’, ‘same use of both
tongues’, ‘mostly Spanish’, and ‘only Spanish’. We took
informants at either extreme of this scale as monolingual
individuals of the corresponding language, and grouped
the central categories as bilinguals. We are interested in
the fractions of speakers in these groups.
IGE polls are stratified by age, which allows us to build
a time-series of fractions of speakers by projecting age
groups in apparent time [24–28] (meaning that the frac-
tion of speakers of people of a certain age become es-
timators of the fraction of speakers when those people
were born). Additionally, data is split into 20 indepen-
dent Galician subregions, each of which is made up of
a collection of smaller counties—but data for individual
counties is not available. Hence, our empirical dataset of
sociolinguistic dynamics consists of 20 time series with
the fractions of monolingual Galician speakers, bilingual
speakers, and monolingual Spanish speakers (Fig. 2a-b).
We base our work on the IGE poll that allowed us of to
estimate the fractions of those born in 2001 [29].
B. Mathematical model
Beginning in the early 90s [30, 31], a growing commu-
nity of mathematicians, physicists, ecologists, and com-
plexity researchers started using systems of differential
equations to model possible trajectories of speakers of co-
existing languages over time [32]. A turning point was the
work by Abrams and Strogatz [33], who fitted their equa-
tions to data from dozens of cohabiting tongues. This
inspired a wave of new models whose stability and dy-
namical classes were thoroughly analyzed [5, 34–44], and
which could in some occasions be tested against empirical
data [4, 28, 44–52].
Different authors would emphasize distinct ingredients
that might affect language coexistence, such as their spa-
tial distribution, or bilingualism (elements that the origi-
nal model by Abrams and Strogatz did not contemplate).
We use one such variation that includes bilingualism [34],
whose stability and dynamics have been studied in detail
[5, 39, 42, 43], and that has been fitted to data of differ-
ent cohabiting languages, including the Galician-Spanish
case [4, 28, 52]. Contrary to some other models with
bilingualism, the one that we use is compatible with ei-
ther the stable coexistence of both tongues, or that one
language takes over and drives the other to extinction.
Thus, the model is agnostic regarding the stability of the
coexisting couple, and the empirical data can constrain
model parameters towards either outcome.
The model consists of a system of coupled differential
equations that tracks the time-evolution of the fraction,
x, of monolinguals of language X (here, Galician); of the
fraction, b, of bilinguals; and of the fraction, y, of mono-
linguals of language Y(here, Spanish). Population is
normalized such that x+y+b= 1, hence two equations
摘要:

AspectrumofcomplexityuncoversDunbar'snumberandotherleapsinsocialstructureMartnSaavedra,1,2JorgeMira,3AlbertoPMu~nuzuri,1,2andLusFSeoane4,51GroupofNonlinearPhysics.UniversidadedeSantiagodeCompostela,15782,SantiagodeCompostela,Spain.2GalicianCenterforMathematicalResearchandTechnology(CITMAga),1578...

展开>> 收起<<
A spectrum of complexity uncovers Dunbars number and other leaps in social structure Mart n Saavedra1 2Jorge Mira3Alberto P Mu nuzuri1 2and Lu s F Seoane4 5.pdf

共13页,预览3页

还剩页未读, 继续阅读

声明:本站为文档C2C交易模式,即用户上传的文档直接被用户下载,本站只是中间服务平台,本站所有文档下载所得的收益归上传人(含作者)所有。玖贝云文库仅提供信息存储空间,仅对用户上传内容的表现方式做保护处理,对上载内容本身不做任何修改或编辑。若文档所含内容侵犯了您的版权或隐私,请立即通知玖贝云文库,我们立即给予删除!
分类:图书资源 价格:10玖币 属性:13 页 大小:1.31MB 格式:PDF 时间:2025-04-30

开通VIP享超值会员特权

  • 多端同步记录
  • 高速下载文档
  • 免费文档工具
  • 分享文档赚钱
  • 每日登录抽奖
  • 优质衍生服务
/ 13
客服
关注